Special Interest Group on Pain in Childhood
Pediatric Pain Letter

:: Home ::
::
Contact ::
::
Current issue ::
::
Past issues ::
::
For authors ::
::
Links ::
::
Search ::

Editor:
Carl L. von Baeyer, PhD
University of Saskatchewan
Saskatoon, Canada

Associate Editor:
Deirdre E. Logan, PhD
Children’s Hospital Boston
Boston, USA

Copyright © 2013,
Special Interest Group on
Pain in Childhood,
International Association
for the Study of Pain®,
www.childpain.org

ISSN 1715-3956

Disclaimer:
Information appearing in Pediatric Pain Letter is not reviewed by, and is not necessarily endorsed by, the Special Interest Group on Pain in Childhood, nor by IASP ®.

Vol. 15 No. 3

December 2013

Commentary

The painful truth: A review of the clinical use of skin conductance monitoring for postoperative pain assessment

Eugene K. Choo

printable version (PDF)

Accurate assessment of pain intensity remains a critical aspect of successful pain management. Currently, the most widely used method of assessment involves various self-report scales (Williamson & Hoggart, 2005). Self-report scales rely on a cooperative, reliable patient and therefore are of limited use with unconscious, preverbal or cognitively impaired individuals. For verbal children, there are over 30 pain scales available, each with their cognitive, social and developmental limitations (Stinson et al., 2006). In preverbal children, pain assessment relies on the use of observational scales to determine pain intensity (von Baeyer & Spagrud, 2007). Limitations of such observational scores include subjectivity, with observations easily confounded by behaviors related to fear or anxiety, and age and context specificity (procedural, postoperative, etc.). Given the numerous limitations of these observational assessments, an objective pain scale that is independent of subjective interpretation certainly sounds attractive. But is it realistic?

The search for an objective measurement of pain is not new. A number of objective measures have been investigated from heart rate variability (Pomfrett, 1999) to even more sophisticated measures such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI; Wartolowska, 2011). Unfortunately, as of yet, none have been able to replace subjective self-report scales.

Skin conductance monitoring has been used in a number of scientific experiments, primarily in the field of psychology (Critchley, 2002). Experiments using galvanic skin responses in order to objectively measure pain have been conducted since the 1950s (Korotkov, 1953; Nisbet et al., 1967; Pakhomov & Ivanov, 1969). A study by Savino et al. (2013) is the most recent in number of studies attempting to use the latest in skin conductance technology as an objective measure of pain.

This commentary will review the clinical use of skin conductance as an objective measure of postoperative pain and discuss the current state of research as well as its limitations.

Background on skin conductance

The underlying theory for the use of skin conductance as a measure of pain is that pain activates the sympathetic nervous system. Activation of the sympathetic neurons associated with the eccrine sweat glands results in perspiration. When an electrical current is applied along the skin’s surface, an increase in conductance can be measured (in microsiemens, µS) in proportion to the amount of sweat produced.

Early studies showed a poor correlation between absolute skin conductance and pain due to wide variability in the amplitude of skin conductance. Therefore current research uses a modified measurement, the number of fluctuations in skin conductance per second (NFSC), which some studies have shown to shown to correlate better with pain assessment (Storm, 2008).

The Med-Storm Pain Monitor System™ (also known as Med-Storm Stress Detector or skin conductance algesimeter; www.med-storm.com) is one of the most commonly used skin conductance monitors currently being used for pain research. The device uses three adhesive electrodes attached to the palmar or plantar surface of the subject to measure skin conductance in real time. Software then integrates the raw data to display various measures such as conductance (µS), or NFSC during a given sampling interval (Storm, 2008). 

Current research

There have been numerous studies on skin conductance throughout the age spectrum of neonates to adults, including during intraoperative and perioperative periods (see Table 1 for details). To date, there are no studies to show that clinical therapy guided by skin conductance measurements improves patient outcomes. The available studies attempt to correlate NFSC with various self-report pain scales. Results have been mixed, with authors describing both positive (Ledowski et al., 2006; Ledowski et al., 2007; Hullett et al., 2009) and negative (Ledowski et al., 2009; Choo et al., 2010; Czaplik et al., 2012) results.

There are numerous possible reasons for the variability in results, which will be discussed further.

Table 1
Summary of selected clinical trials using skin conductance fluctuations to assess pain in awake postoperative patients

Study

Ledowski et al., 2006

Ledowski et al., 2007

Hullett et al., 2009

Ledowski et al., 2009

Choo et al., 2010

Czaplik et al., 2012

N

25 adults

75 adults

165 children

100 adults

90 children

44 adults

Age range (years)

21-67

19-81

1-16

18-32

7-17

>18

Self-report pain scale cut-off

NRS >3

NRS >3

VAS >3

NRS >5

NRS >3

NRS >2

NFSC Cut-off

0.10

0.10

0.13

0.10

0.2

>0.13

Sensitivity

89.2%

88.5%

90.4%

58%

51.9%

77.9%

Specificity

73.5%

67.7%

64.5%

61%

78.4%

41.2%

Sampling period

Not reported

5s

15s

5s

60s

60s

AUC

Not reported

75.5%

82%

Not reported

62.3%

Not reported

Correlation (r)

0.625

0.477

Not reported

0.165

0.21

0.571

Note. NRS = Numerical Rating Scale. VAS = Visual Analog Scale. AUC = Area under receiver operator curve. Correlation = correlation between NFSC and self-report pain scale.

Sampling interval. As previously mentioned, there is a poor correlation between pain and absolute skin conductance and thus the NFSC during a given sampling interval is used. In general, most of the studies with positive results have used a very short sampling interval of 5-15 seconds, whereas studies using longer sampling intervals of 30-60 seconds have produced negative results. This is surprising, as one would expect clinically significant pain that warrants intervention to last longer than 60 seconds. This may suggest that artifacts, which are likely to have a bigger influence in a shorter sampling interval, may influence the final measure.

Cut-offs. The manufacturer recommended cut-offs for the Med-Storm Pain Monitor are shown in Table 2.

Table 2
Pain intensity scores assumed to be associated with values of NFSC, from the Med-Storm Pain Monitor™ User Manual, p. 22.

Number of fluctuations in skin conductance
per second (NFSC)
Corresponding pain intensity score
(Visual Analog Scale, 0-10)

0.00-0.07

No pain

0.13-0.21

1-3

0.27

4-5

0.33

6-8

0.40-0.70

8-10

Note. No directions are given for interpretation of overlapping or missing values.

These values appear to be loosely based on the study by Ledowski et al. (2007). The various studies have used different cut-off values for NFSC to suggest different levels of clinically significant levels of pain. Lowering the cut-off value improves sensitivity but sacrifices specificity.

Confounding variables. Initial studies of the NFSC variable suggested that it was fairly robust, being resistant to external factors such as external temperature, neuromuscular blockade and adrenergic agonists (Storm, 2008). However it can be influenced by a number of confounders discussed below.

Movement artifact. Movement artifacts have repeatedly been reported to be a significant confounder (Storm, 2008). This could partially explain the observation of poor results with longer measurement times. However, this poses a significant practical problem in being able to obtain a reliable measurement in a noncooperative, nonverbal patient in pain.

Temperature. A study by Valkenburg et al. (2012) found a direct correlation between skin temperature and NFSC in the absence of clinically significant pain, suggesting that skin temperature autoregulation could confound skin conductance results in some settings.

General arousability response. Given the numerous functions of the sympathetic nervous system, it is not surprising that activation can occur from numerous stimuli unrelated to pain. It was noted by Czaplik et al. (2012) that NFSC values seem to fluctuate quite frequently, making it difficult to identify and document a predominant value. They also reported that increases in NFSC were observed by simply speaking to the patient to assess pain intensity. Choo et al. (2010) also reported a significant increase in NFSC during subject responses to pain scale assessment, again suggesting a general arousability response. This certainly could explain the observations of Savino et al. (2013) where a significant rise in NFSC occurred during heel lancing, but then promptly returned to baseline.

Objective measurements for subjective symptoms

Over fifty years ago, Henry K. Beecher, anesthesiologist and pioneer in pain research and medical ethics, noted the challenges associated with developing objective measures of subjective phenomena in clinical practice (Berde & McGrath, 2009). Perhaps there is another reason that a reliable objective measure of pain has not yet been discovered. All of the current research of objective measures of pain use self-report scales as the standard for comparison and thus can only at best show equivalence. If an objective scale cannot be shown to be superior, can it be shown to be more reliable? Here again, the available evidence is lacking and as we have discussed, there are a number of confounders that compromise its reliability.

The International Association for the Study of Pain (Merskey et al., 1994) states that pain is always a subjective experience. If we accept that pain is a subjective experience, then arguably it seems logical that it would be best assessed with a subjective measure whenever possible.

Conclusions

There is currently a lack of evidence to recommend the use of skin conductance for the assessment of pain. Due to the variability in results, it would not be prudent to use skin conductance to guide therapeutic interventions for pain at this time. Further research and optimization to identify an optimal sampling interval, minimize the influence artifacts caused by movement and general arousal are needed to improve the specificity of skin conductance as a measure of pain. An objective measure of pain remains elusive and will likely remain so for quite some time.

Eugene K. Choo, BScPharm, MD
Department of Anesthesiology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, ON, Canada
email: echoo[at]toh.on.ca

Cite as: Choo EK. The painful truth: A review of the clinical use of skin conductance monitoring for postoperative pain assessment. Pediatric Pain Letter 2013;15(3):29-33. www.childpain.org/ppl 

Acknowledgements

Drs. Carolyne J. Montgomery MD, FRCPC and J. Mark Ansermino MBBCh, FRCPC, for their guidance and assistance in reviewing this paper and the Department of Pediatric Anesthesiology, British Columbia Children’s Hospital.

References

Berde C, McGrath P. Pain measurement and Beecher’s challenge: 50 years later. Anesthesiology 2009;111:473-474. PubMed Abstract

Choo EK, Magruder W, Montgomery CJ, Lim J, Brant R, Ansermino JM. Skin conductance fluctuations correlate poorly with postoperative self-report pain measures in school-aged children. Anesthesiology 2010;113:175-182. PubMed Abstract

Critchley HD. Electrodermal responses: what happens in the brain. Neuroscientist 2002;8:132-142. PubMed Abstract

Czaplik M, Hübner C, Köny M, Kaliciak J, Kezze F, Leonhardt S, et al. Acute pain therapy in postanesthesia care unit directed by skin conductance: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 2012;7:e41758. PubMed Abstract

Hullett B, Chambers N, Preuss J, Zamudio I, Lange J, Pascoe E, Ledowski T. Monitoring electrical skin conductance: a tool for the assessment of postoperative pain in children? Anesthesiology 2009;111:513-517. PubMed Abstract

Korotkov LA. Objective registration of pain manifestations by means of galvanometry. Sov Med 1953;17:15-17. 

Ledowski T, Bromilow J, Paech MJ, Storm H, Hacking R, Schug SA. Monitoring of skin conductance to assess postoperative pain intensity. Br J Anesth 2006;97:862-865. PubMed Abstract

Ledowski T, Bromilow J, Wu J, Paech MJ, Storm H, Schug SA. The assessment of postoperative pain by monitoring skin conductance: results of a prospective study. Anaesthesia 2007;62:989-993. PubMed Abstract

Ledowski T, Ang B, Schmarbeck T, Rhodes J. Monitoring of sympathetic tone to assess postoperative pain: skin conductance vs surgical stress index. Anesthesia 2009;64:727-731. PubMed Abstract

Med-Storm Innovation AS. Med-Storm Pain Monitor™ User Manual. Version 1.0 English, 2012. Link

Merskey H, Bogduk N, IASP Task Force on Taxonomy. Classification of chronic pain: descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms (2nd ed.). Seattle: IASP Press, 1994. Link

Nisbet HIA, Norris W, Brown J. Objective measurement of sedation IV: the measurement and interpretation of electrical changes in the skin. Br J Anesth 1967;39:798-805. PubMed Abstract

Pakhomov VI, Ivanov EV, Kol's OR. Skin resistance as an objective test of pain sensitivity. Eksp Khir Anesteziol 1969;14:35-36.

Pomfrett CJ. Heart rate variability, BIS and ‘depth of anaesthesia’. Br J Anaesth 1999;82:659-662. PubMed Abstract

Savino F, Vagliano L, Ceratto S, Viviani F, Miniero R, Ricceri F. Pain assessment in children undergoing venipuncture: the Wong-Baker faces scale versus skin conductance fluctuations. PeerJ 2013;1:e37 PubMed Abstract

Stinson JN, Kavanagh T, Yamada J, Gill N, Stevens B. Systematic review of the psychometric properties, interpretability and feasibility of self-report pain intensity measures for use in clinical trials in children and adolescents. Pain 2006;125:143-157. PubMed Abstract

Storm H. Changes in skin conductance as a tool to monitor nociceptive stimulation and pain. Curr Opin
Anaesthesiol 2008; 21:796-804. PubMed Abstract

Valkenburg AJ, Niehof SP, van Dijk M, Verhaar EJ, Tibboel D. Skin conductance peaks could result from changes in vital parameters unrelated to pain. Pediatr Res 2012;71:375-379. PubMed Abstract

von Baeyer CL, Spagrud LJ. Systematic review of observational (behavioral) measures of pain for children and adolescents aged 3 to 18 years. Pain 2007;127:140-150. PubMed Abstract

Williamson A, Hoggart B. Pain: a review of three commonly used pain rating scales. J Clin Nurs 2005;14:798-804. PubMed Abstract

Wartolowska K. How neuroimaging can help us to visualise and quantify pain? Eur J Pain 2011;5(Suppl 2):323-327.